American Sign Language (ASL)

Position Statement

Ad Hoc Committee to Establish a Position on American Sign Language (ASL)

About This Document: This position statement is an official policy of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA). ASHA develops position statements when the Association's official stance on a topic needs to be clearly established for members and the public. Although ASHA has long recognized that ASL is a language, the request to draft this statement arose from inconsistencies in how federal agencies view ASL as a language distinct from English for the purposes of service provision (see, e.g., National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, 2019; National Science Foundation, 2019; U.S. Department of Education, 2011). This statement formally clarifies and affirms ASHA's long-held position that ASL is a distinct language.

In August 2018, the ASHA Board of Directors (BOD) approved a resolution to form the Ad Hoc Committee to Establish a Position on ASL (hereafter, "the Committee"). The resolution charged that the Committee consist of six ASHA members with expertise and experience regarding ASL. The Committee included a member who is deaf, an ASL linguist, audiologists, speech-language pathologists, and a teacher of the deaf. The Committee members who developed this position statement were James Mahshie, chair; Katie Brennan; Tina Childress; Cheryl DeConde Johnson; Brenda Seal; and Aaron Shield. Adena Dacy served as ex officio. Marie Ireland, Vice President for Speech-Language Pathology Practice (2018–2020) served as the BOD liaison. The statement was open for peer review by all interested parties prior to final approval by the BOD. Respondents included audiologists, speech-language pathologists, linguists, teachers of the deaf, ASL teachers, interpreters, researchers, administrators, other related professionals and students, professional associations, individuals who are D/deaf, families, and advocacy groups.

Table of Contents

Position Statement: American Sign Language (ASL)

The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) affirms that American Sign Language (ASL) is a language, possessing complex levels of language organization, including phonology, morphology, semantics, syntax, and pragmatics. ASL is distinct from other signed languages around the world and from English. Like all languages, ASL evolves over time within specific historical, social, and cultural contexts.

Rationale

Introduction

ASL is a distinct, rule-governed language that has existed in the United States and parts of Canada for more than 200 years. ASL consists of linguistically specified handshapes, locations, movements, palm orientations, and non-manual markers to convey information (Valli, Lucas, Mulrooney, & Rankin, 2011). As with any language, ASL has social, ethnic, and geographic variations and dialects. ASL linguistics has evolved as a specific research discipline that is the object of systematic study in graduate courses and doctoral programs in universities throughout the United States and Canada. 

Historical Background

ASL emerged in the 19th century at what is now called the American School for the Deaf (ASD), founded in Hartford, Connecticut, in 1817 by Thomas Hopkins Gallaudet and Laurent Clerc (Lane, Pillard, & French, 2000; Supalla & Clark, 2015). The roots of ASL can be traced to a mixture of Old French Sign Language, which was the language of instruction at ASD, and the signed languages and systems in use in 18th and 19th century New England (e.g., Martha's Vineyard Sign Language; Groce, 1985; Lane & Grosjean, 2010; Lane et al., 2000; Padden, 2010; Supalla & Clark, 2015). Professor William Stokoe of Gallaudet College's Linguistics Research Lab first documented ASL as a language in 1960 (Stokoe, 1960). Stokoe, along with his colleagues Carl Croneberg and Dorothy Casterline, recognized the linguistic structure of ASL in the book Dictionary of American Sign Language on Linguistic Principles (Stokoe, Casterline, & Croneberg, 1965).

Equivalence of Signed and Spoken Languages

Linguistic, developmental, and neurobiological research confirms that there is equivalence between signed and spoken languages (Emmorey et al., 2005; Emmorey & McCullough, 2009; Klima & Bellugi, 1979; Kovelman, Shalinsky, Berens, & Petitto, 2014; Stokoe, 1960; Stokoe, Armstrong, Karchmer, & Van Cleve, 2002). Children who are exposed early to ASL achieve language milestones on the same trajectory as children who acquire spoken languages (Anderson & Reilly, 2002; Emmorey, 2002; Meier & Newport, 1990; Newport & Meier, 1985). Moreover, the "critical period" of language acquisition applies to signed and spoken languages alike, such that the ability to learn a first language decreases with age, and late-exposed learners show age-of-acquisition effects (Mayberry, 1993; Mayberry & Eichen, 1991; Newport, 1990). The left-hemisphere language centers of the brain associated with comprehension and production of spoken languages are also involved in the comprehension and production of ASL and other signed languages (Campbell, MacSweeney, & Waters, 2008; Emmorey et al., 2005; Hickok, Bellugi, & Klima, 1996, 1998; Kovelman et al., 2014; Kassubek, Hickok, & Erhard, 2004; MacSweeney et al., 2002; MacSweeney, Capek, Campbell, & Woll, 2008; Petitto et al., 2000).

Autonomy of the Linguistic System

ASL is an autonomous linguistic system independent from English, from other signed and spoken languages, and from Manually Coded English systems, such as Signing Exact English (Allard & Chen Pichler, 2018; Pfetzing, Zawolkow, & Gustason, 1972). Like all languages, ASL possesses its own rules of phonology (Brentari, 1992; Petitto et al., 2016; Sandler, 1989), morphology (Allard & Chen Pichler, 2018; Aronoff, Meir, & Sandler, 2000, 2005; Padden, 1988), and syntax (Allard & Chen Pichler, 2018; Chen Pichler, 2002; Liddell, 1980; Lillo-Martin & Chen Pichler, 2006; Neidle, Kegl, MacLaughlin, Bahan, & Lee, 2000). Like all languages, ASL also has conventions for formal and informal registers as well as rules for turn-taking and for initiating, maintaining, and changing communication topics (Holcomb, 2013; Mindess, 2014; Supalla & Clark, 2015; Wilbur, 2006; Wilbur & Petitto, 1983). 

Conclusion

ASHA's affirmation that ASL is a distinct language is consistent with ASHA's 1982 document titled Language, which designates that

  • language is a complex and dynamic system of conventional symbols that is used in various modes for thought and communication;
  • language evolves within specific historical, social, and cultural contexts;
  • language, as rule-governed behavior, is described by at least five parameters—phonologic, morphologic, syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic;
  • language learning and use are determined by the interaction of biological, cognitive, psychosocial, and environmental factors; and
  • effective use of language for communication requires a broad understanding of human interaction, including such associated factors as nonverbal cues, motivation, and sociocultural roles.

References

Allard, K., & Chen Pichler, D. (2018). Multi-modal visually-oriented translanguaging among Deaf signers. Translation and Translanguaging in Multilingual Contexts, 4, 384–404.

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1982). Language [Relevant Paper]. Available from www.asha.org/policy/.

Anderson, D., & Reilly, J. (2002). The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory: Normative data for American Sign Language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 7, 83–106.

Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2000). Universal and particular aspects of sign language morphology. In K. K. Grohmann & C. Struijke (Eds.), University of Maryland Working Papers in Linguistics (Vol. 10, pp. 1–33).

Aronoff, M., Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2005). The paradox of sign language morphology. Language, 81, 301–344.

Brentari, D. (1992). Phonological representation in American Sign Language. Language, 68, 359–374.

Campbell, R., MacSweeney, M., & Waters, D. (2008). Sign language and the brain: A review. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, 3–20.

Chen Pichler, D. (2002). Word order variation and acquisition in American Sign Language. Sign Language & Linguistics, 5, 89–97.

Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Emmorey, K., Grabowski, T., McCullough, S., Ponto, L. L. B., Hichwa, R. D., & Damasio, H. (2005). The neural correlates of spatial language in English and American Sign Language: A PET study with hearing bilinguals. Neuroimage, 24, 832–840.

Emmorey, K., & McCullough, S. (2009). The bimodal bilingual brain: Effects of sign language experience. Brain and Language, 109, 124–132.

Groce, N. E. (1985). Everyone here spoke sign language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hickok, G., Bellugi, U., & Klima, E. (1996, June 20). The neurobiology of sign language and its implications for the neural basis of language. Nature, 381, 699–702.

Hickok, G., Bellugi, U., & Klima, E. S. (1998). What's right about the neural organization of sign language? A perspective on recent neuroimaging results. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,2, 465–468.

Holcomb, T. (2013). Introduction to American Deaf culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Kassubek, J., Hickok, G., & Erhard, P. (2004). Involvement of classical anterior and posterior language areas in sign language production, as investigated by 4 T functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience Letters, 364, 168–172.

Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., Berens, M. S., & Petitto, L. A. (2014). Words in the bilingual brain: An fNIRS brain imaging investigation of lexical processing in sign-speech bimodal bilinguals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–11.

Lane, H. L., & Grosjean, F. (2010). Recent perspectives on American sign language. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Lane, H. L., Pillard, R., & French, M. (2000). Origins of the American deaf-world: Assimilating and differentiating societies and their relation to genetic patterning. Sign Language Studies, 1, 17–44.

Liddell, S. (1980). American Sign Language syntax. The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton.

Lillo-Martin, D., & Chen Pichler, D. (2006). Acquisition of syntax in signed languages. In B. Schick, M. Marschark, & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Advances in the sign-language development of deaf children (pp. 231–261). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.

MacSweeney, M., Capek, C. M., Campbell, R., & Woll, B. (2008). The signing brain: the neurobiology of sign language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 432–440.

MacSweeney, M., Woll, B., Campbell, R., McGuire, P. K., David, A. S., Williams, S. C., . . . Brammer, M. J. (2002). Neural systems underlying British Sign Language and audio‐visual English processing in native users. Brain, 125, 1583–1593.

Mayberry, R. I. (1993). First-language acquisition after childhood differs from second-language acquisition: The case of American Sign Language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36, 1258–1270.

Mayberry, R. I., & Eichen, E. B. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 486–512.

Meier, R. P., & Newport, E. L. (1990). Out of the hands of babes: On a possible sign advantage in language acquisition. Language, 66, 1–23.

Mindess, A. (2014). Reading between the signs: Intercultural communication for sign language interpreters (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Intercultural Press.

National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. (2019). American Sign Language. Retrieved from  https://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/american-sign-language

National Science Foundation. (2019). Sign languages. Retrieved from https://www.nsf.gov/news/special_reports/linguistics/sign.jsp

Neidle, C., Kegl, J., MacLaughlin, D., Bahan, B., & Lee, R. G. (2000).The syntax of American Sign Language: Functional categories and hierarchical structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14, 11–28.

Newport, E. L., & Meier, R. P. (1985). The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 1, pp. 881–938). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Padden, C. (1988). Interaction of morphology and syntax in American sign language. New York, NY: Garland Press.

Padden, C. (2010). Sign language geography. In G. Mathur & D. Napoli (Eds.), Deaf around the world (pp. 19–37). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Petitto, L. A., Langdon, C., Stone, A., Andriola, D., Kartheiser, C., & Cochran, C. (2016). Visual sign phonology: Insights into human reading and language from a natural soundless phonology. WIREs Cognitive Science, 7, 366–381.

Petitto, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E. J., Dostie, D., & Evans, A. C. (2000). Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: Implications for the neural basis of human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 13961–13966.

Pfetzing, G., Zawolkow, D., & Gustason, E. (1972). Signing Exact English: Seeing instead of hearing. Los Alamitos, CA: Modern Signs Press.

Sandler, W. (1989). Phonological representation of the sign: Linearity and nonlinearity in American Sign Language. Dordrecht, South Holland, the Netherlands: Foris Publications.

Stokoe, W. C. (1960). A dictionary of American Sign Language on linguistic principles. Burtonsville, MD: Linstok Press.

Stokoe, W. C., Armstrong, D. F., Karchmer, M. A., & Van Cleve, J. V. (2002). The study of signed languages: Essays in honor of William C. Stokoe. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

Stokoe, W.C., Casterline, D., & Croneberg, C. (1965). A dictionary of American Sign Language on linguistic principles. Washington, DC: Gallaudet College.

Supalla, T., & Clark, P. (2015). Sign language archaeology: Understanding the historical roots of American Sign Language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

United States Department of Education. (2011, January 27). Title III guidance letter to the director: American Sign Language. Retrieved from https://www2.ed.gov/programs/sfgp/americansignlang.pdf

Valli, C., Lucas, C., Mulrooney, K. J., & Rankin, M. N. P. (2011). Linguistics of American Sign Language: An introduction (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

Wilbur, R. (2006). Sign language: Discourse and pragmatics. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language & linguistics (2nd ed., pp. 303–307). Oxford, United Kingdom: Elsevier Science.

Wilbur, R. B., & Petitto, L. A. (1983). Discourse structure In American Sign Language conversations (or, how to know a conversation when you see one). Discourse Processes, 6, 225–228.

Reference this material as: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2019). American Sign Language [Position Statement]. Retrieved from www.asha.org/policy/


© Copyright 2019 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. All rights reserved.


Disclaimer: The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association disclaims any liability to any party for the accuracy, completeness, or availability of these documents, or for any damages arising out of the use of the documents and any information they contain.


ASHA policy documents contain information for use in all settings; however, members must consider all applicable local, state, and federal requirements when applying the information in their specific work setting.


doi:10.1044/policy.PS2019-00354

ASHA Corporate Partners